This review appeared in Intelligence in 2012.
The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence. Edited by Robert J.
Sternberg and Scott Barry Kaufman. Cambridge University Press, New York, NY.
ISBN 978-0-521-73911-5. XIX, 885 pages.
A Sternberg handbook on intelligence is a welcome part of
any psychologist’s library, and a new volume arouses pleasurable anticipation.
Weighing in at 1.7 Kilos and almost a thousand pages, it is a small library in
itself, guaranteeing that there will be much to enjoy and reflect upon. Of the 84 authors only 12 are not from the USA
and Canada. Four of that dozen are from Britain, 2 from Singapore, 1 from New
Zealand, 1 from Australia (incorrectly classified as USA) 3 from
Austria/Germany, and 1 from Russia, though she is also half-time in the USA.
From the above it can be deduced that the cleverest people in the world come
from Britain and its former colonies, a clear vindication of its lingering
sense of exceptionalism. The result is a
pleasing preponderance of Anglo-Saxon empiricism, tempered by a moiety of
Teutonic perfectionism. However, the majority of individual examples,
legislative frameworks and historical references are US centric, 312 million
citizens being given precedence over the other 7 billion. It is natural to search
for truth where the light is brightest, and the grants most generous, but the
results and the frames of mind may lack representativeness.
Residence in a nation state need not blind the authors to
the global panoply of human accomplishment, so perhaps the deeper analysis is
to turn to the back of the book and ask: “Who do these authors admire”? I list
in ascending order the total number of pages on which the most popular authors
are mentioned, for brevity setting 20 pages as the minimal entrance
qualification: K.W.Fisher 21, R.E.Nisbett 21, C.J. De Young 21, P.Salovey 22 , O.Wilhelm
22, A.Binet 23, J.R.Flynn 23, R.D.Roberts 23, A.S.Kaufman 24, A.R.A.Conway 25, D.K.Detterman
25, K.A.Ericsson 25, J.R.Gray 25, S.B.Kaufman 26, D.Wechsler 26, J.D.Mayer 28, R.W.Engle
29, R.B.Cattell 34, P.L.Ackerman 35, S.J.Ceci 35, J.L.Horn 35, J.B.Carroll 38, C.Spearman
41, K.E.Stanovich 44, E.L.Grigorenko 46, A.R.Jensen 48, H.Gardner 58, I.J.Deary
71, R.J.Sternberg 156.
Is the list representative of the most accomplished and
impactful thinkers? Readers will have their own preferences, and can compare
the list with the usual citation indexes, but my own reaction is that (all artefacts
aside) there are several names likely to gain general approval, some absences,
and several surprises. Daniel Kahneman,
our one psychologist/economist Nobel Laureate does not make the cut, meriting
11 page mentions. There is a paper in this for someone.
Finally, one has to ask the indelicate question: “How
intelligent are the authors”? None of them should flinch from this evaluation,
but should metaphorically sit down next to thousands of test takers and welcome
it as a contribution to knowledge. It is evident that all of them have vast
vocabularies, which suggest exceptionally high intellects. However, they appear
weak in visuo-spatial skills. There are a few path analyses, some exemplars of
test and experimental materials, a very few drawings, some developmental scales
and tiers, and one sliced and labelled brain. The scarcity of illustration
would shame any Upper Paleolithic cave. There is little to help a hard-pressed
lecturer throw together some teaching slides. Perhaps there were cost constraints.
The authors’ numerical skills are harder to evaluate. Some have included a few
tables with numbers, the others, perhaps wisely, have not. Numbers may turn out to be wrong. The end
result (with honourable exceptions) is proof of the power and the limitations
of words, and testimony to psychology’s lack of precision: all those hours of
psychometric testing and data analysis resulting in a tentative “perhaps”. It
is instructive that the latest findings in intelligence can be communicated
with few models and numbers, in a way that genetics or physics would find difficult.
Lastly, believers in multiple intelligences will understand that I was unable
to judge the authors in terms of their musical, naturalistic, interpersonal, bodily-kinaesthetic,
or indeed gastro-intestinal accomplishments.
Despite this necessarily unrepresentative sample of high
achievers, there are many good chapters in this volume, with enough clarity,
enthusiasm, novelty and reflection to engender intelligent thought in the
reader. A mere sampling, in quasi-random order: Zentall’s explanations of
cognitive dissonance and gambling in pigeons; Gaborra and Russon’s evolutionary history of
intelligence, which is like paging through an ancient family photograph album,
waiting for one’s own likeness to emerge; Nettlebeck on the basic processes of reaction and inspection
times, confirming their moderate links with intelligence(0.3 to 0.5); Conway et
al. making a case (0.5) for working memory tapping processes shared with fluid
intelligence; Niu and Brass on the Confucian path to knowledge “some by natural
ease, some by desire of its advantages, some by strenuous effort, but the
achievement being made, it comes to the same thing”; Halpern et al. “on
average, women and men live systematically different lives”; Flynn’s lucid
prose partly illuminating the mystery of secular intelligence gains (some
well-chosen numbers here) arguing that modernity has taught us to cope with
abstraction (though the gains for the ultimate abstraction of mathematics are the
smallest, and baby tests show the same overall secular gains, so it cannot all
be due to the availability of heuristics); Suzuki et al. provide the better
chapter on race and intelligence, though hereditarians may feel insufficiently
represented; Barnett et al. discuss the effects of intelligence on national
economies and give Rindermann’s betas for each finding; Deary et al. sit on top
of the psychometric gold mine of population-wide longitudinal samples from
whence the bullion of new results regularly flow, in this instance on intelligence’s
astounding effects on health and lifespan; De Young on personality, saying of
the relation between intelligence and openness to experience that(quoting
Saucier) “Intelligence perturbs the
orbit of any construct that comes near to it”, and to give final word to a 6
year old child writing to his mentor in Feldman and Morelock’s chapter on
prodigies and savants “I am working on a
unified theory. Are you? My unified theory is broken up into many parts, each
part the size of special relativity. I really know my geometry… A
rhombicosidodecahedron is the largest known polyhedron. It is huge!” Humans have spectacular intelligence.
There is strength in depth, and much to enjoy, but also some
lapses. Some authors “correct” group
ability differences for socio-economic-status without considering that these economic
differences may be due in large part to prior intelligence. Cross-lagged
designs in longitudinal studies may offer methodologically superior results. I
would have liked a strong comparative study of the predictive utility of
psychometrics-as-usual versus multiple-intelligences with respect to the full
range of intelligence on a range of real world variables. Some theoretical chapters placed stakes in
the ground, but the foundations had yet to solidify. “Giftedness” seems heterogeneous
and poorly defined. Some authors could have benefitted from reading Earl Hunt’s
epilogue, particularly on stereotype threat and the non-existence of race, and
then re-considering their arguments.
So, do these intelligent and conscientious authors deserve
to pass? Apart from some minor typographical errors, this examiner would
recommend that, with some rebalancing of most-quoted authors and the revision
of a few chapters, and a thorough factor-analytic pruning of the multiple intelligences
section, the candidates should be encouraged to proceed to the next volume.
James Thompson
If handbooks got degrees - sound upper Lower Second, I'd say.
ReplyDeleteFew fireworks, and much huffing and puffing about multiple intelligences. Interestingly, they have replied to both reviews in Intelligence, defending their choice of quoted authors as those who deserve attention because they are pushing beyond "g" but who also believe in it, and besides, "g" is well established and does not need further support.
ReplyDelete"g" is well established and does not need further support: well quite. Writing a handbook of physics, who would bother to mention the work of Newton, Faraday, Clerk Maxwell, Planck or Einstein?
ReplyDeleteQuite. One could also dispense with Rutherford, Galileo, Bohr, Cavendish and JJ Thomson. Even Dirac would not get a look in.
ReplyDelete