After broadcasting some terrible news the TV presenter in the studio would
turn to me and say: Can I have a reaction?
What sort of reaction? Should I burst into tears, faint, or perhaps punch
him on the nose for juxtaposing a human tragedy with a banal question? The
interviewer, of course, wants a thought, a comment, or an observation: instant words of wisdom cast in the form of a
spontaneous reply. They want something more than a mere reaction, at the speed
of a reflex.
Perhaps, without
intending it, they are making a fundamental point. The essence of a living
organism is its ability to react to its environment. We test the injured and
unconscious for their reactions. The extremely useful Glasgow Coma Scale (Teasdale
and Jennet 1974) was created to measure the level of consciousness after head
injury, and is now widely used in emergency medicine and intensive care. When I
was doing my thesis on brain damaged children in the late sixties we simply
recorded the period of unconsciousness, without specifying how that was established.
The scale is very simple, and is based on motor reactions (“from makes no
movements” to “responds to commands”) verbal production (“makes no sounds” to “converses
normally”) and those windows into the soul, the eyes (“does not open eyes” to “opens
eyes spontaneously”).
So, if we have the misfortune to be severely injured, the
decision to switch off the machine keeping us “alive” will be based on our
reactions, though nowadays that includes the reactions that can be determined
by brain scans alone. I think, therefore
I am.
However, in ordinary healthy life we can do more than just
react. We can react fast. Survival
depends upon it. Had I the power, dear reader, to make the chair you are
sitting in drop instantly by a mere one inch, you would show an almost instant
startle response. In about a quarter of a second you would find that the
message from your bottom (no chair present) was much more interesting than even
my riveting comments. Your eyes would have widened in surprise, your arms
extended and you would be ready for action, so ready that you would probably
have stopped all the non-essential bodily systems, and have prepared yourself for
extreme action. Useful things, rapid reactions.
How fast can we
react? There are many ways to measure one’s reactions. The BBC has a reaction time test amusingly
based on firing tranquilizer darts into sheep:
but there are many others, and this one is based on a
traffic light, which has more face validity:
This morning my reactions (on the sheep test) have taken an
average of 0.29 seconds, and the best I have achieved in the last two weeks is
0.2128
Before going further with the decimal points, I should warn
you that my results were less good when using the standard laptop keyboard
(perhaps the keys are getting worn out through over-use) and much quicker with
a new wireless mouse, which is probably far more sensitive. So, be aware of measurement
errors, and find the most sensitive system you can.
However, I can take only partial comfort from my reaction
times, because my main interest is determining how quickly I can react as a driver. These computer game test
results have to be corrected because of a historical decision taken at the
beginning of the motor car age. Early horse drawn coaches had hand operated
brakes, though they were hardly immediate. The operator had to wind a handle
which applied the brake to the wheel. Apart from handle, the main “brake” was
also hand operated, in the sense of the alarmed coachman pulling back on the
horse’s reins. Much quicker, but not particularly effective if the coach was
racing downhill.
When designers of horseless carriages tried to decide where
to put all the controls on the new-fangled motor car, they found that drivers
had their hands full with the steering wheel and the gears. Also, to get the braking
effect it was far better for the driver to step with full force on the mechanical
pedal. Hence the brake pedal was put on
the floor, where it has remained.
With the rise of servo-assisted systems that placement was almost
irrelevant by the eighties, but cultural habits tend to persist. Most studies of driver’s reaction times show
that foot/pedal reaction times at about 0.4 or 0.5 are twice as long as
hand/button reaction times. To make things
worse, even those times are achieved only when drivers know that their
reactions are about to be measured. Real life emergencies come without warning.
Under the more realistic conditions of sustained driving, it takes about 1.2
seconds for drivers to move their foot from accelerator to brake pedal.
It is time to provide
a brake function on the steering wheel. The most ecologically valid system would
probably be to measure the force with which drivers push down on the steering
wheel when trying to stop, which is part of the startle response and the wish
to push back from the approaching hazard. It would be necessary to avoid false
activations of the brake system, but that can be worked on by adjusting the
sensitivity of the system. In brief, the emergency stop function must come back
to the hand, where reactions are faster.
Away from the
peculiarities of the motor car, what can be made of simple reaction times? A great deal, seems to be the answer. It some
ways it is one of the world’s simplest IQ tests. See a target: respond. Not too
intellectually challenging. The task is easy to teach to just about any
conscious person, without any fancy instructions. It is easy to administer rewards,
encouraging even those with learning difficulties to participate. It is a real
scale with a real zero, and every microsecond counts as much as any other
microsecond, which in S.S.Steven’s (1946) theory of scale types is a ratio scale, the
most quantitative of quantitative scales.
So, how well does reaction time predict intelligence?
Correlations of about 0.3 are found for simple reaction time, and a larger 0.49 for more complex choice reaction tasks. Why should the faster reactors be
brighter, and the brighter participants faster? One simple unifying theme is
that if you have a faster central processor you can solve simple problems
(reaction time tasks) faster, and solve complex problems to a higher standard
because you have more mental time to work through the solutions.
Faster reactors also
live longer. It may be that they have solved the problem of living: don’t
smoke, don’t overeat, don’t drink too much, and don’t take stupid risks.
Perhaps so. Yet the most recent research suggests that, helpful as intelligence
may be in life, it is not directly responsible for extending lifespans. Much of
the intriguing correlation between intelligence and lifespan can be explained
by simple reaction times. It seems that if you have fast reactions you
probably have a good nervous system, which means you probably have other bodily
systems which are good as well. You have System
Integrity. You are like a car which was made on a good day: all systems are
present and correct, because all the operators were paying attention to their
assembly tasks.
So, armed with my reaction times, I will now try to calculate
my remaining lifespan. This may take some time, because searching the relevant
literature for the predictive equations takes longer than shooting a dart at a
BBC sheep.
and the best I have achieved in the last two weeks is 0.2128
ReplyDeleteInteresting. my reaction time is a pathetic 0.33 ms. I hope Woodley is wrong that reaction time correlates to g at -.6!
Practice, practice! First thing to note is that you may be getting an artefactual delay due to the mouse clicker, etc. The other thing is that there is a practice effect to a minor degree (Woodley covers this) in that all proper testing allows the subject several trials so that they can get used to the equipment.
DeleteDoubtful - when I took an IAT (I have a 'strong implicit bias' btw) I was tripping over the keyboard, and that's a pretty good CRT.
Delete