Words are our greatest tools. They allow us to
guide, warn, praise and admonish each other and to cooperate in complex tasks.
Even restricting ourselves to oral traditions, we can pass down stories which
preserve some of the wisdom of our ancestors. Once we master the art of reading
and writing we can supersede the restrictions of memory and draw on the wisdom
of successive generations. Knowledge is power, and the accumulated knowledge of
many centuries is the most powerful of all.
Vocabulary acquisition is an enthralling process to
study, as children learn the rules of grammar, and sometimes apply them with
greater consistency than the idiosyncrasies of actual spoken adult language
would deem correct, if you have gotted the point. Children may refer to little
rodents as “mouses” because children have noticed a language rule, and applied
it correctly. The English language is not always correct, for idiosyncratic and
historical reasons. The English who goed West on the Mayflower were full of
reformist zeal, and managed to improve the unwritten constitution by writing it
down, and improved writing by removing some of the colourful miss-spellings of
the English language and by making spelling more logical and less colorful, but
they did not get round to a full purging of languistic errors and exceptions,
or at the very least they have not gotten round to it yet (this is a recondite
joke, because all English speakers at that time said “gotten” and then with
time the English moved to “got” for the past tense, no doubt just to aggravate
the colonials).
Some people have the simplistic notion that
vocabulary must be determined by mere exposure to spoken language. That is
necessary, but far from sufficient, as even children work out. They notice
patterns, informal rules, and the contexts in which communication takes place. “The acquisition of meaning is based on the
eduction of meaning from the contexts in which the words are encountered”. (So,
even if the word “eduction” in the quotation from page 146 of Jensen’s “Bias in
mental Testing” is unfamiliar, you will not be surprised to deduce that it
means “To assume or work out from given facts;
deduce”). The meaning of a word is acquired in some contexts which
permits at least some partial inference as to its meaning. By hearing or
reading the word in different contexts, through a process of generalization,
discrimination and eduction one can guess at the essence of the meaning of the
word, so as to use it (experimentally) oneself the next time a similar context
presents itself. Words move from being unfamiliar
to familiar, from familiar but not really understood to
being familiar and partly understood (at
which stage the explanations given about the meaning of the word are threadbare
and inaccurate), and from there to
being explained by use of synonyms (though
those can range from partial to full understanding as shown by power of the
explanations and definitions).
Testing vocabulary precisely is
quite complicated, because you have to test how well subjects really understand
the words in question. It is a bit like trying to find out whether people can
really handle heavy machinery, as opposed to boasting about it. Typically
vocabulary tests work up from common to rare words, and specify what sorts of
definitions and explanations will get full points. On multiple choice
questions, the use of distractor items often reveals that many people have misunderstood
the meanings of words that are new to them. For example, some people who think
they know how to define FATUOUS are distracted by the option large. An argument may be witless, silly
and pointless, but not obese.
A very short vocabulary test, which
correlates 0.71 with IQ, is the ten word test in the General Social Survey (US).
Can something so crude yield interesting results? Yes. Razib Khan has a very
informative post on this. In my view, no survey should be conducted without
including a test like this, which provides a very good estimate of
intelligence.
A stab at using this test to calculate the intellectual
demands of particular jobs is provided by The Audacious Epigone. Note the low
score for academics.
Nonetheless, long vocabulary tests,
or dynamic computer—administered tests which adapt quickly to your difficulty
level provide reasonable estimates of your total word store. In that sense,
intelligence ranges from 0 to 45,000 words (the real upper limit if one avoids
technical jargon), and one can put a single number on it, on a proper ratio
scale. Rating people by the size of their word stores makes sense. Although 3000 words will provide a great deal
of cost-effective and very useful communication, additional words bring
conceptual benefits. Knowledge of the 3000 most frequent words in the English language
will probably result in your understanding 95% of what is said to you, and
knowledge of 5000 “word families” (the main word and its variants, like quick: quickly, quicker, quickest) should
mean that you would be able to understand 99.9%. Why have more? The answer is
that much of good thinking depends on a powerful vocabulary. Carpentry can be
done with a standard tool set, cabinet making requires finer, more specialised
tools.
As a rough guide, teenagers have
about 12,000 words and college students 17,000. Older adults have 17,000 to
21,000 words, and a minority have many more. Some conceited person referred to 20,000 words
as being “the incoherence boundary”. I eschew such contemptuous judgements.
Bright children acquire vocabulary
faster than duller children, and thus brighter adults have larger vocabularies,
because they require fewer contexts to work out word meanings, and make more
subtle discriminations in meanings between similar words. In 1962 Alice Heim,
to whom a statue should be raised somewhere, designed a new test of verbal
reasoning called “The word in context”. Charming and intriguing, it featured an
unfamiliar foreign word in a descriptive paragraph. Subsequent paragraphs gave
more descriptive context, until the elusive meaning was potentially resolved. A
brilliant idea, but the test took too long for it to be used in psychometry.
Most crucially, bright people often
observe things and have thoughts about things before they learn the words they
need to express them. They have the need before the word, so that when the word
comes into view they seize on it with pleasure, and relief. Such words are
treasured, and stick in the mind because of their elegant utility. Words for
which we have no need are shapeless, and never stick in the mind.
So, a word is not just something we
have heard, like a bird-song. A word is a cog in a meaning machine. To learn a
word is to mine the essence of a context, to condense a cloud of implications
into the condensate of definition.
You may wish to discuss this post with someone you
love. Be warned: even in autochthonous pairings, discrepancies in terpsichorean
accomplishments can lead to uxoricide.
Do not be captious after reading this post.
"Note the low score for academics." I wonder. To get into the top two streams in my secondary school we had to have an IQ of 118, or so we were told.
ReplyDeleteCog, cogwheel or sprocket? I think we should be told.
ReplyDeleteI believe someone used the Heim test with race differences and found none whatsoever (Intelligence, 2009 I think). Why would the Heim test be too long to use in psychometric batteries? Even a 4-hour-long group intelligence test (like the SAT) is easier to use than a 1-hour-individually-proctored test, and the Heim test seems like it could be used without proctoring.
ReplyDelete"Vocabulary: humanity’s greatest achievement?"
ReplyDeleteIt's between that and the comeback boomerang.
General reply: academics ought to be bright, in line with the respect they crave, but the range is wide, from IQ 115 upwards. A heterogeneous population.
ReplyDeleteThe Alice Heim "Word in context" was fascinating, but highly inefficient. Even in paper and pencil group tests one should be efficient so as to maximise concentration and avoid fatigue effects. Nowadays with dynamic computer based testing the whole approach has improved. The big time waster in intelligence testing is that people vary enormously in intelligence. One is always trying to find the correct range in which to pitch the items.
More important than the vocabulary size, is its functionality, is to express in a text or in everyday life.
ReplyDeleteThe philosopher's stone of intelligence is self-didactism, ie, the intrinsic desire to learn.
Santoculto
Vocabulary comprehension is a crucial component in acquiring reading and comprehension skills.
ReplyDeleteVocabulary is one of the most important component of English Language. Vocabulary can be enhanced by playing vocabulary games, learning new words and applying them in daily life.This can be done on www.vocabmonk.com.