Monday 9 December 2013

Intelligence in 2000 words

 

I have noticed that in my series of 4 posts “All you ever wanted to know about intelligence (but were too bright to ask)” the first part got far more attention than the others, so I have put them together and boiled them down to 1,985 words.

“Some people are cleverer than others. It is a prominent and consistent way in which people differ from each other; the measurements we make of people’s cleverness produce scores that are correlated with important life outcomes; it is interesting to discover the mechanisms that produce these individual differences; and understanding these mechanisms might help to ameliorate those states in which cognitive function is low or declining.”

Deary identifies four major sources of scepticism about intelligence:

1 The concept appears to be too general. People argue that they are better at some skills than others, and assume different modules are involved, such that we are all good at some specific mental skill.

2 Historical events in intelligence research which are discreditable. In the UK, the 11+ missed out people who later showed demonstrable talent; cases of probable fraud in reporting results; over use and over-interpretation of intelligence tests; controversies about intelligence differences between ethnic groups; or claims that “ordinary” intelligence has now been replaced by tests of “multiple” intelligence.

3 “It is possible that clever people develop a kind of cognitive noblesse oblige; they kind of know they have won the lottery on a valuable trait, but they think it is bad form to acknowledge it.”

4 They probably haven’t read good quality research on the topic.

I find that most of the hostility about intelligence comes from bright people, who keep up with the broad sweep of newspaper reports and popular books, but have not looked at good quality research. Despite this lack they are surprisingly vehement when they ridicule IQ. It would appear that intelligence is most disparaged by the intelligent. “Define intelligence” they demand, with a knowing smile.

There are three types of answer: a quip, an explanation, and a formula.

The Quip: “Intelligence is what you need when you don’t know what to do”. Carl Bereiter coined this elegant phrase. It captures the ultimate purpose of intelligence, which is to help you cope with the unknown. The best intelligence test is the puzzle to which no one knows the answer. For example, is there a detectable particle which gives objects mass?

The Explanation: “Intelligence is a very general mental capability that, among other things, involves the ability to reason, plan, solve problems, think abstractly, comprehend complex ideas, learn quickly and learn from experience. It is not merely book learning, a narrow academic skill, or test-taking smarts. Rather, it reflects a broader and deeper capability for comprehending our surroundings — ‘catching on,’ ‘making sense’ of things, or ‘figuring out’ what to do.” Linda Gottfredson and 52 leading psychometricians agree with this explanation. http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/reprints/1997mainstream.pdf

The formula: g+group+specific skill+error, where g accounts for about 50% of the variance. (I have written this in English, but it should be displayed in eigenvalues).

clip_image001

So, let us look behind the formula “In 1904 Spearman found that people who perform well on one type of cognitive test tend to perform well on others. That is, if cognitive test scores are ordered so that better performance equals a higher score, the correlations between them are all positive. There is shared variation among all types of cognitive performance. Spearman called this shared/common variance g: an abbreviation for general intelligence. In the 100+ years since then, every study that has applied a diverse battery of cognitive tests to a decent-sized group of people with a mix of ability levels has re-discovered the same thing: there is some cognitive variance shared by all cognitive tests. Typically, if one applies principal components analysis, just under half of the total test score variance is accounted for by the first unrotated principal component.”

This finding of 50% of the variance in ability being due to g is matched by another finding:  IQ-type test scores are highly reliable, and highly stable. For example, when the same intelligence test is taken at age 11 years and repeated at almost 80, about 50% of the variance is stable.

So, half of intelligence is due to a common factor, and half of the variance is stable throughout life.

What are the causes of differences in intelligence? Twin and adoption studies show that intelligence is about 50% heritable. Counter-intuitively, heritability estimates increase to 80% as you age and put many years between yourself and your family’s influence on you. It looks like genes take time to express themselves, or set in motion processes which take time to develop.

If you measure a diverse range of skills you can show that g is highly heritable, but there is less genetic influence that is specific to each domain. Heritabilities are probably higher in richer people, and lower in deprived groups (suggesting the environment has most effect when it is very bad).

There is no “gene for intelligence” with the exception of the APOE gene, where individuals with one or two e4 alleles tend to have lower ability in old age, and declining cognition across their lifetimes.

Genome wide assessment studies haven’t come up with very much, yet. There is some molecular genetic evidence that some variance in intelligence is detected by single nucleotide polymorphisms. Applying genetic complex trait analysis, between a quarter and a half of intelligence variance can be accounted for by variants in linkage disequilibrium with common SNPs. This analysis cannot identify the precise causal genes. It suggests that intelligence is highly polygenic, with large numbers of variants of small effect sizes.

The genetic correlation between intelligence measured in childhood and old age in the same individual is high: to a substantial extent the same genes cause higher intelligence in both childhood and old age. All these studies require very large sample sizes (125,000) and allow major risks to be computed: in individuals who are at genetic risk for schizophrenia, but have not exhibited the disorder, cognitive ability in old age is lower than in those without schizophrenic genetic risk.

With regards to the much vaunted effects of the environment, twin studies suggest that the contribution of the shared environment (family for example) to intelligence is small to negligible by adulthood, and the remaining variance is due to individually created environments and error.

There is a general finding that there is a modest correlation of 0.30 between intelligence test scores and brain size, and a similarly sized correlation between intelligence and the general integrity of the brain’s white matter, as measured by diffusion tensor MRI. Sample sizes here are of the order of 500 persons. The association is largely accounted for by people’s differences in speed of processing. Cleverer brains seem to be more efficient. Consequently, they can think about complex problems faster and for longer, thus being more likely to find solutions.

Education

”People who score better on intelligence tests tend to stay longer in education, to gain higher-level qualifications, and to perform better on assessments of academic achievement. Some of the correlations between intelligence scores at the end of primary school and academic results some years later are high, suggesting that it is not just a matter of education boosting intelligence. Also, educational attainment has a moderately high heritability, and a strong genetic correlation with intelligence. On the other hand, there is also evidence that education can provide a boost to scores on tests of complex thinking, and some of these increments last into
old age. Therefore, there is probably a bidirectional causal association between intelligence and education.”

Social status and mobility

“People who score better on intelligence tests tend to go into more professional occupations (typically those with higher status) and to perform better
in the workplace. There is a positive association between intelligence test
scores in childhood and social position later in life: people who score higher
tend to be in more professional jobs, to live in less deprived areas, and to
have higher incomes.”
This is not due to people stating their intelligence test results in their job applications nor, usually, to employers conducting their own intelligence tests (though that would often be useful). These results are obtained by looking at historical intelligence test results years after the children have grown up and finished their professional careers. 

“The association is not perfect. Results show that, when it comes to attained social position in maturity, intelligence, education and parental background all count to
some extent. That is, there is some meritocracy and intelligence-driven social mobility, and there is also some social inertia.”

Intelligence seems to have a cumulative effect, and relates more strongly to occupational and social position later rather than earlier in adulthood.

Intelligence and health

Intelligence is associated with better health and longer lifespans, but it is not entirely clear why. The early explanation was that more intelligent people learned quickly how to avoid health hazards. They gave up smoking sooner in life, bothered to read the medicine labels, and followed health advice generally. Now it seems possible that both intelligence tests and life itself test a general underlying bodily system integrity, a fundamental mens sana in corpore sana which, if you are lucky enough to have it, gives you health, intelligence and long life without much exertion on your part. Typical, isn’t it, that evolution doesn’t understand human concepts of fairness and equity? Also typical that many very good papers were written showing how intelligent people avoided health hazards, and now it turns out that those will have to be re-written.

Age related cognitive decline

“There are declines in cognitive function even among people who do not develop dementia. Not all cognitive functions decline at the same rate. Some cognitive functions — often referred to as markers of crystallized intelligence — hold up well with age. These include vocabulary and general and specific knowledge. The cognitive functions that tend to decline are called fluid intelligence. These tend to involve on-the-spot thinking with novel materials, and in situations in which past knowledge is of limited help. This includes abstract reasoning, spatial abilities, processing speed, and working and other types of memory.

Not everyone experiences the same rate of cognitive decline, and there is a growing interest in the genetic and environmental (biological and social) determinants of people’s differences in age-related cognitive changes. Some of the more solid evidence exists for the following being cognitively protective: not having the APOE e4 allele, being physically more active and fit, and not smoking.”

Two main hypotheses are: some people have a “cognitive reserve” such that their brains are better able to withstand damage, perhaps because a bigger brain provides redundancy or because some people’s brains are more flexible in reorganizing networks to regain or retain cognitive functions; or the common system hypothesis that age-related decline of different bodily systems is correlated; that people who are experiencing faster cognitive declines might also be experiencing faster declines in sensory and some physical functions, making researchers consider inflammation, oxidative stress, telomere length, and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis as common causes of variance.

Finally, Deary goes on to discuss the “so-named Flynn effect, whereby the absolute scores on intelligence tests have been rising since testing started in the early-to-mid 20th century. The extent of the rise, its geographical distribution in the world, and especially its causes are all still being studied. Some hypothesise that better nutrition might explain some of the increase, and others put it down to society’s making more accessible and emphasizing the skills tested by intelligence tests.”

“On the biological side there is research showing that breast feeding is associated with a sizeable advantage in intelligence later in childhood. However, there is also some evidence that this is explained by the higher intelligence scores of the mothers who tend to breastfeed.

“Adoption from a deprived to a more affluent setting is reported to be associated
with an intelligence advantage. There is still debate about the effectiveness
of intensive intervention programmes early in life, and whether any cognitive
advantages last or whether advantage accrues to social rather than cognitive
skills.”

Deary ends with a plea: “Human intelligence is important; it matters in our lives.”

Here are his references:

Deary, I.J. (2012). Intelligence. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 63, 453–482.
Deary, I.J., Penke, L., and Johnson, W. (2010). The neuroscience of human intelligence differences.Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 201–211 .
Hunt, E. (2011 ). Human Intelligence (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Nisbett, R. E., Aronson, J., Blair, C., Dickens, W., Flynn, J., Halpern, D. F., and Turkheimer, E. (2012). Intelligence: new findings and theoretical developments. Am. Psychol. 67, 503–504.
Salthouse, T.A. (2010). Major Issues in Cognitive Ageing (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

15 comments:

  1. "many very good papers were written showing how intelligent people avoided health hazards …": since medical fashion in identifying health hazards lurches all over the place, there must be limits to the very possibility of this, save at the level of the bleeding' obvious - don't grip live wires, and that sort of thing.

    ReplyDelete
  2. Ironically, the first time I ever heard about the correlation between sperm quality & good looks, was at university.

    The professor gave the reasoning: "over time, evolution selects for good qualities. These tend to be selected for and therefore good qualities will tend to concentrate in the same individuals."

    It made total sense to me, while I was somewhat annoyed by his fatalist statement at the time (19 at the time). Not just me, most of the students thought he was just joking around. I did realize after a few hours that we he said had to be true. At least, if you believe evolution to be true.

    One caveat though, diseases in your youth do disrupt natural development/ maturing processes. There's a link between good immune system & beauty. This could lead to less symetrical (facial) features in adulthood which are deemed less attractive. A clean environment, of course, exposes an individual to fewer of these stressors and thus 'prettier'/'handsomer' results. An unclean environment (= many pathogens, diseases) makes it harder for good looks to be widespread. In such environments good looks do correspond 101 with good immune systems.

    (Unclean environments are naturally also the result of a more intelligent population too.. So, it's all feedback loop.)

    -- Calisthene

    ReplyDelete
  3. What is the evolutionary point of acne?

    ReplyDelete
  4. Success. Acne keeps going, so it is achieving evolutionary success.

    ReplyDelete
  5. Well-meaning-amateur9 December 2013 at 18:09

    "4 They probably haven’t read good quality research on the topic."

    I suspect that this is the main cause. In the U.S., at least, Gould cemented the mainstream narrative with "The Mismeasure of Man." The periodic pillorying of figures such as James Watson and Charles Murray continues to keep everyone in line. If a curious person wishes to know what the data actually support, he must purposefully seek that out. The Internet is making the task easier, however, QED.


    ReplyDelete
    Replies
    1. Yes, we have to reclaim the day, so that data can be read in public.

      Delete
  6. OK; what is the evolutionary point of humans being susceptible to acne?

    ReplyDelete
  7. None. Evolution doesn't have a point. Acne is successful, people with acne less so. Parasite/host relationships are one and the same thing: success in an environment. There is no point.

    ReplyDelete
  8. Mr. Thompson,

    I have a general question about language proficiency.

    Why are some people much more articulate than others?

    Could there really be a gene tied to something as specific as grammar?

    ReplyDelete
  9. First, some people are much more capable on particular mental tasks than others, so it is usual to have differences between people in their proficiencies. Being articulate is one example among many. Although there are instances in which a specific gene is associated with a specific disorder, most mental skills are created by the actions of many, many genes, each having a small individual effect, but coming together to make significant differences between people. So, there aren't really specific genes for grammar, or humour, or intelligence. That said, sometime researchers latch on to a particular gene as a key indicator, or litmus for a change in the evolutionary development of abilities. You could try reading about the FOXP2 gene, but remember my cautionary remarks as you do so.

    ReplyDelete
    Replies
    1. I think the most accurate definition of intelligence should be the ability to acquire information, understand it and then apply or manipulate it. There are obviously numerous kinds of information such as spatial, verbal, social and emotional information. So I think we should also give more significance to tests that measure EI (emotional intelligence) and Social intelligence.

      All of the current IQ tests do not measure intelligence correctly. There are different mental processes that have to be measured in in intelligence testing that IQ tests do not do. For example short term memory, long term memory, working memory, attention span and executive function. These mental capacities also involve different networks during Spatial and Verbal information. Therefore it's almost impossible to capture all these cognitive processes correctly in a single written testing.

      Delete
  10. Keep up the excellent work , I read few articles on this site and I think that your web blog is real interesting and Power to the People of excellent information.

    ReplyDelete
  11. Excellent information and facts. Only real difficulty I was basically receiving was viewing the pics. No idea exactly why.

    ReplyDelete
  12. Great ?V I should certainly pronounce, impressed with your website. I had no trouble navigating through all tabs as well as related info ended up being truly simple to do to access. I recently found what I hoped for before you know it in the least. Reasonably

    ReplyDelete
  13. This the very effective for us ..so i get a good site to get information

    acne research

    ReplyDelete